{br} STUCK with your assignment? {br} When is it due? {br} Get FREE assistance. Page Title: {title}{br} Page URL: {url}
UK: +44 748 007-0908, USA: +1 917 810-5386 [email protected]
  1. .QUESTION

     In the article “What is Beautiful is Good,” why was it assumed, of all the characteristics in their favor, that physically attractive people would be poorer parents than unattractive or average attractive people?    

    Discussion Board #2 Directions:

    Be sure to use the material from the reading to answer the question (do not use the internet)

    I. Answer the Following Question:

    1.) In the article “What is Beautiful is Good,” (please find article attached) why was it assumed, of all the characteristics in their favor, that physically attractive people would be poorer parents than unattractive or average attractive people?

    Directions for Response:
    a.) Response must be academically substantive

    b.) Response must be a minimum of 150 words and a maximum of 300 words.

    c.) Response must be checked for spelling and grammar

    d.) Direct quotes must be used sparingly and placed in quotation marks

    III. Create 1 original critical thinking question from your reading assignment.

    a.) The critical thinking question must be original, academically substantive, and thought provoking.\

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    19722,. Vol. 24, No. 3, 285-290

    WHAT IS BEAUTIFUL IS GOOD

    KAREN DION,2 ELLEN BERSCHEIDs

    University of Minnesota

    ELAINE WALSTER

    University of Wisconsin

    A person’s physical appearance, along with his sexual identity, is the personal

    characteristic that is most obvious and accessible to others in social interaction.

    The present experiment was designed to determine whether physically

    attractive stimulus persons, both male and female, are (a) assumed to possess

    more socially desirable personality traits than physically unattractive stimulus

    persons and (6) expected to lead better lives (e.g., be more competent

    husbands and wives, be more successful occupationally, etc.) than unattractive

    stimulus persons. Sex of Subject X Sex of Stimulus Person interactions

    along these dimensions also were investigated. The present results indicate a

    “what is beautiful is good” stereotype along the physical attractiveness dimension

    with no Sex of Judge X Sex of Stimulus interaction. The implications of

    such a stereotype on self-concept development and the course of social interaction

    are discussed.

    A person’s physical appearance, along with

    his sexual identity, is the personal characteristic

    most obvious and accessible to others in

    social interaction. It is perhaps for this reason

    that folk psychology has always contained

    a multitude of theorems which ostensibly

    permit the forecast of a person’s character

    and personality simply from knowledge

    of his outward appearance. The line of deduction

    advanced by most physiognomic

    theories is simply that “What is beautiful is

    good . . . [Sappho, Fragments, No, 101],”

    and that “Physical beauty is the sign of an

    interior beauty, a spiritual and moral beauty

    . . . [Schiller, 1882].”

    Several processes may operate to make the

    soothsayers’ prophecies more logical and accurate

    than would appear at first glance.

    First, it is possible that a correlation between

    inward character and appearance exists because

    certain personality traits influence

    one’s appearance. For example, a calm, relaxed

    person may develop fewer lines and

    wrinkles than a tense, irritable person. Second,

    cultural stereotypes about the kinds of

    personalities appropriate for beautiful or ugly

    people may mold the personalities of these

    1 This research was financed in part by National

    Institute of Mental Health Grants MH 16729 to

    Berscheid and MH 16661 to Walster.

    2 Now at the University of Toronto.

    8 Requests for reprints should be sent to Ellen

    Berscheid, Laboratory for Research in Social Relations,

    University of Minnesota, Elliott Hall, Minneapolis,

    Minnesota SS4SS.

    individuals. If casual acquaintances invariably

    assume that attractive individuals are more

    sincere, noble, and honest than unattractive

    persons, then attractive individuals should be

    habitually regarded with more respect than

    unattractive persons. Many have noted that

    one’s self-concept develops from observing

    what others think about oneself. Thus, if the

    physically attractive person is consistently

    treated as a virtuous person, he may become

    one.

    The above considerations pose several questions:

    (a) Do individuals in fact have stereotyped

    notions of the personality traits possessed

    by individuals of varying attractiveness?

    (6) To what extent are these stereotypes

    accurate? (c) What is the cause of the

    correlation between beauty and personality if,

    in fact, such a correlation exists?

    Some observers, of course, deny that such

    stereotyping exists, and thus render Questions

    b and c irrelevant. Chief among these

    are rehabilitation workers (cf. Wright, 1960)

    whose clients possess facial and other physical

    disabilities. These researchers, however,

    may have a vested interest in believing that

    physical beauty is a relatively unimportant

    determinant of the opportunities an individual

    has available to him.

    Perhaps more interestingly, it has been

    asserted that other researchers also have had

    a vested interest in retaining the belief that

    beauty is a peripheral characteristic. Aronson

    (1969), for example, has suggested that the

    285

    286 K. DION, E. BERSCHEID, AND E. WALSTER

    fear that investigation might prove this assumption

    wrong has generally caused this to

    be a taboo area for social psychologists:

    As an aside, I might mention that physical attractiveness

    is rarely investigated as an antecedent of

    liking—even though a casual observation (even by

    us experimental social psychologists) would indicate

    that we seem to react differently to beautiful women

    than to homely women. It is difficult to be certain

    why the effects of physical beauty have not been

    studied more systematically. It may be that, at some

    levels, we would hate to find evidence indicating

    that beautiful women are better liked than homely

    women—-somehow this seems undemocratic. In a

    democracy we like to feel that with hard work and

    a good deal of motivation, a person can accomplish

    almost anything. But, alas (most of us believe),

    hard work cannot make an ugly woman beautiful.

    Because of this suspicion perhaps most social psychologists

    implicitly prefer to believe that beauty is

    indeed only skin deep—and avoid the investigation

    of its social impact for fear they might learn otherwise

    [p. 160].

    The present study was an attempt to determine

    if a physical attractiveness stereotype

    exists and, if so, to investigate the content

    of the stereotype along several dimensions.

    Specifically, it was designed to investigate

    (a) whether physically attractive stimulus

    persons, both male and female, are assumed

    to possess more socially desirable personality

    traits than unattractive persons and

    (b) whether they are expected to lead better

    lives than unattractive individuals. With respect

    to the latter, we wished to determine if

    physically attractive persons are generally

    expected to be better husbands and wives,

    better parents, and more successful socially

    and occupationally than less attractive persons.

    Because it seemed possible that jealousy

    might attenuate these effects (if one is jealous

    of another, he may be reluctant to accord the

    other the status that he feels the other deserves),

    and since subjects might be expected

    to be more jealous of attractive stimulus persons

    of the same sex than of the opposite

    sex, we examined the Sex of Subject X Sex of

    Stimulus Person interactions along the dimensions

    described above.

    METHOD

    Subjects

    Sixty students, 30 males and 30 females, who were

    enrolled in an introductory course in psychology at

    the University of Minnesota participated in this experiment.

    Each had agreed to participate in return for

    experimental points to be added to their final exam

    grade.

    Procedure

    When the subjects arrived at the designated rooms,

    they were introduced to the experiment as a study

    of accuracy in person perception. The experimenter

    stated that while psychological studies have shown

    that people do form detailed impressions of others

    on the basis of a very few cues, the variables determining

    the extent to which these early impressions

    are generally accurate have not yet been completely

    identified. The subjects were told that the

    purpose of the present study was to compare person

    perception accuracy of untrained college students

    with two other groups who had been trained in

    various interpersonal perception techniques, specifically

    graduate students in clinical psychology and

    clinical psychologists. The experimenter noted his

    belief that person perception accuracy is a general

    ability varying among people. Therefore, according

    to the experimenter, college students who are high on

    this ability may be as accurate as some professional

    clinicians when making first-impression judgments

    based on noninterview material.

    The subjects were told that standard sets of photographs

    would be used as the basis for personality

    inferences. The individuals depicted in the photographs

    were said to be part of a group of college

    students currently enrolled at other universities who

    were participating in a longitudinal study of personality

    development scheduled to continue into adulthood.

    It would be possible, therefore, to assess the

    accuracy of each subject’s judgments against information

    currently available on the stimulus persons

    and also against forthcoming information.

    Stimulus materials. Following the introduction,

    each subject was given three envelopes. Each envelope

    contained one photo of a stimulus person of

    approximately the subject’s own age. One of the

    three envelopes that the subject received contained

    a photograph of a physically attractive stimulus

    person; another contained a photograph of a person

    of average attractiveness; and the final envelope

    contained a photograph of a relatively unattractive

    stimulus person.4 Half of our subjects received three

    pictures of girls; the remainder received pictures of

    boys.

    4 The physical attractiveness rating of each of the

    pictures was determined in a preliminary study.

    One hundred Minnesota undergraduates rated SO

    yearbook pictures of persons of the opposite sex

    with respect to physical attractiveness. The criteria

    for choosing the 12 pictures to be used experimentally

    were (a) high-interrater agreement as to the physical

    attractiveness of the stimulus (the average interrater

    correlation for all of the pictures was .70);

    and (b) pictures chosen to represent the very attractive

    category and very unattractive category were

    not at the extreme ends of attractiveness.

    WHAT is BEAUTIFUL is GOOD 287

    To increase the generalizability of our findings and

    to insure that the general dimension of attractiveness

    was the characteristic responded to (rather than

    unique characteristics such as hair color, etc.), 12

    different sets of three pictures each were prepared.

    Each subject received and rated only 1 set. Which

    1 of the 12 sets of pictures the subject received, the

    order in which each of the three envelopes in the

    set were presented, and the ratings made of the

    person depicted, were all randomly determined.

    Dependent variables. The subjects were requested

    to record their judgments of the three stimulus persons

    in several booklets.5 The first page of each

    booklet cautioned the subjects that this study was

    an investigation of accuracy of person perception

    and that we were not interested in the subjects’

    tact, politeness, or other factors usually important

    in social situations. It was stressed that it was important

    for the subject to rate the stimulus persons

    frankly.

    The booklets tapped impressions of the stimulus

    person along several dimensions. First, the subjects

    were asked to open the first envelope and then to

    rate the person depicted on 27 different personality

    traits (which were arranged in random order).”

    The subjects’ ratings were made on 6-point scales,

    the ends of which were labeled by polar opposites

    (i.e., exciting-dull). When these ratings had been

    computed, the subject was asked to open the second

    envelope, make ratings, and then open the third

    envelope.

    In a subsequent booklet, the subjects were asked

    to assess the stimulus persons on five additional

    personality traits.7 These ratings were made on a

    slightly different scale. The subjects were asked to

    indicate which stimulus person possessed the “most”

    and “least” of a given trait. The stimulus person

    thought to best represent a positive trait was assigned

    a score of 3; the stimulus person thought to

    possess an intermediate amount of the trait was

    assigned a score of 2; and the stimulus person

    thought to least represent the trait was assigned a

    score of 1.

    6 A detailed report of the items included in these

    booklets is available. Order Document No. 01972

    from the National Auxiliary Publication Service of

    the American Society for Information Science, c/o

    CCM Information Services, Inc., 909 3rd Avenue,

    New York, New York 10022. Remit in advance

    $5.00 for photocopies or $2.00 for microfiche and

    make checks payable to: Research and Microfilm

    Publications, Inc.

    fl The subjects were asked how altruistic, conventional,

    self-assertive, exciting, stable, emotional, dependent,

    safe, interesting, genuine, sensitive, outgoing,

    sexually permissive, sincere, warm, sociable,

    competitive, obvious, kind, modest, strong, serious,

    sexually warm, simple, poised, bold, and sophisticated

    each stimulus person was.

    7 The subjects rated stimulus persons on the following

    traits: friendliness, enthusiasm, physical attractiveness,

    social poise, and trustworthiness.

    In a previous experiment (see Footnote 5), a

    subset of items was selected to comprise an index of

    the social desirability of the personality traits assigned

    to the stimulus person. The subjects’ ratings

    of each stimulus person on the appropriate items

    were simply summed to determine the extent to

    which the subject perceived each stimulus person as

    socially desirable.

    In order to assess whether or not attractive persons

    are expected to lead happier and more successful

    lives than unattractive persons, the subjects were

    asked to estimate which of the stimulus persons

    would be most likely, and which least likely, to have

    a number of different life experiences. The subjects

    were reminded again that their estimates would

    eventually be checked for accuracy as the lives of

    the various stimulus persons evolved. The subjects’

    estimates of the stimulus person’s probable life experiences

    formed indexes of the stimulus person’s

    future happiness in four areas: (a) marital happiness

    (Which stimulus person is most likely to ever

    be divorced?); (,6) parental happiness (Which stimulus

    person is most likely to be a good parent?);

    (c) social and professional happiness (Which stimulus

    person is most likely to experience deep personal

    fulfillment?); and (d) total happiness (sum of Indexes

    a, b, and c ) .

    A fifth index, an occupational success index, was

    also obtained for each stimulus person. The subjects

    were asked to indicate which of the three stimulus

    persons would be most likely to engage in 30 different

    occupations. (The order in which the occupations

    were presented and the estimates made was

    randomized.) The 30 occupations had been chosen

    such that three status levels of 10 different general

    occupations were represented, three examples of

    which follow: Army sergeant (low status) ; Army

    captain ‘ (average status); Army colonel (high

    status). Each time a high-status occupation was

    foreseen for a stimulus person, the stimulus person

    was assigned a score of 3; when a moderate status

    occupation was foreseen, the stimulus person was

    assigned a score of 2; when a low-status occupation

    was foreseen, a score of 1 was assigned. The average

    status of occupations that a subject ascribed to a

    stimulus person constituted the score for that stimulus

    person in the occupational status index.

    RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

    Manipulation Check

    It is clear that our manipulation of the

    relative attractiveness of the stimulus persons

    depicted was effective. The six unattractive

    stimulus persons were seen as less attractive

    than the average stimulus persons, who, in

    turn, were seen as less attractive than the six

    attractive stimulus persons. The stimulus

    persons’ mean rankings on the attractiveness

    dimension were 1.12, 2.02, and 2.87, respec288

    1. DION, E. BERSCHEID, AND E. WALSTER

    TABLE 1

    TRAITS ATTRIBUTED TO VARIOUS STIMULUS OTHERS

    Trait ascription”

    Social desirability of

    the stimulus person’s

    personality

    Occupational status

    of the stimulus person

    Marital competence

    of the stimulus person

    Parental competence

    of the stimulus person

    Social and professional

    happiness of the

    stimulus person

    Total happiness of

    the stimulus person

    Likelihood of marriage

    Unattractive

    stimulus

    person

    56.31

    1.70

    .37

    3.91

    5.28

    8.83

    1.52

    Average

    stimulus

    person

    62.42

    2.02

    .71

    4.55

    6.34

    11.60

    1.82

    Attractive

    stimulus

    person

    65.39

    2.25

    1.70

    3.54

    6.37

    11.60

    2.17

    ” The higher the number, the more socially desirable, the

    more prestigious an occupation, etc., the stimulus person Is

    expected to possess.

    tively. These differences were statistically significant

    (F= 939.32 ).8

    Test of Hypotheses

    It will be recalled that it was predicted

    that the subjects would attribute more socially

    desirable personality traits to attractive individuals

    than to average or unattractive individuals.

    It also was anticipated that jealousy

    might attenuate these effects. Since the subjects

    might be expected to be more jealous of

    stimulus persons of the same sex than of the

    opposite sex, we blocked both on sex of subject

    and sex of stimulus person. If jealousy

    attenuated the predicted main effect, a significant

    Sex of Subject X Sex of Stimulus

    Person interaction should be secured in addition

    to the main effect.

    All tests for detection of linear trend and

    interaction were conducted via a multivariate

    analysis of variance. (This procedure is outlined

    in Hays, 1963.)

    The means relevant to the hypothesis that

    attractive individuals will be perceived to

    possess more socially desirable personalities

    than others are reported in Table 1. Analyses

    reveal that attractive individuals were indeed

    8 Throughout this report, d/=l/55,

    judged to be more socially desirable than are

    unattractive (F = 29.61) persons. The Sex of

    Subject X Sex of Stimulus Person interaction

    was insignificant (interaction F = .00).

    Whether the rater was of the same or the

    opposite sex as the stimulus person, attractive

    stimulus persons were judged as more socially

    desirable.9

    Furthermore, it was also hypothesized that

    the subjects would assume that attractive

    stimulus persons are likely to secure more

    prestigious jobs than those of lesser attractiveness,

    as well as experiencing happier marriages,

    being better parents, and enjoying

    more fulfilling social and occupational lives.

    The means relevant to these predictions

    concerning the estimated future life experiences

    of individuals of varying degrees of

    physical attractiveness are also depicted in

    Table 1. As shown in the table, there was

    strong support for all of the preceding hypotheses

    save one. Attractive men and women

    were expected to attain more prestigious occupations

    than were those of lesser attractiveness

    (F = 42.30), and this expectation was

    expressed equally by raters of the same or the

    opposite sex as the stimulus person (interaction

    .F= .25).

    The subjects also assumed that attractive

    individuals would be more competent spouses

    and have happier marriages than those of

    lesser attractiveness (^ = 62.54). (It, might

    be noted that there is some evidence that this

    may be a correct perception. Kirkpatrick and

    9 Before running the preliminary experiment to

    determine the identity of traits usually associated

    with a socially desirable person (see Footnote 5),

    we had assumed that an exciting date, a nurturant

    person, and a person of good character would be

    perceived as quite different personality types. Conceptually,

    for example, we expected that an exciting

    date would be seen to require a person who was

    unpredictable, challenging, etc., while a nurturant

    person would be seen to be predictable and unthreatening.

    It became clear, however, that these

    distinctions were not ones which made sense to the

    subjects. There was almost total overlap between

    the traits chosen as representative of an exciting date,

    of a nurturant person, and a person of good or

    ethical character. All were strongly correlated with

    social desirability. Thus, attractive stimulus persons

    are assumed to be more exciting dates (F = 39.97),

    more nurturant individuals (F = 13.96), and to have

    better character (F = 19.57) than persons of lesser

    attractiveness.

    WHAT is BEAUTIFUL is GOOD 289

    Cotton ( 1 9 5 1 ) , reported that “well-adjusted”

    wives were more physically attractive than

    “badly adjusted” wives. “Adjustment,” however,

    was assessed by friends’ perceptions,

    which may have been affected by the stereotype

    evident here.)

    According to the means reported in Table

    1, it is clear that attractive individuals were

    not expected to be better parents (F = 1.47).

    In fact, attractive persons were rated somewhat

    lower than any other group of stimulus

    persons as potential parents, although no

    statistically significant differences were apparent.

    As predicted, attractive stimulus persons

    were assumed to have better prospects for

    happy social and professional lives (F =

    21.97). All in all, the attractive stimulus persons

    were expected to have more total happiness

    in their lives than those of lesser attractiveness

    (F= 24.20).

    The preceding results did not appear to be

    attenuated by a jealousy effect (Sex of Subject

    X Stimulus Person interaction Fs = .01,

    .07, .21, and .05, respectively).

    The subjects were also asked to estimate

    the likelihood that the various stimulus persons

    would marry early or marry at all. Responses

    were combined into a single index.

    It is evident that the subjects assumed that

    the attractive stimulus persons were more

    likely to find an acceptable partner than

    those of lesser attractiveness (F = 35.84).

    Attractive individuals were expected to marry

    earlier and to be less likely to remain single.

    Once again, these conclusions were reached by

    all subjects, regardless of whether they were

    of the same or opposite sex of the stimulus

    person (interaction F — .01).

    The results suggest that a physical attractiveness

    stereotype exists and that its content

    is perfectly compatible with the “What is

    beautiful is good” thesis. Not only are physically

    attractive persons assumed to possess

    more socially desirable personalities than

    those of lesser attractiveness, but it is presumed

    that their lives will be happier and

    more successful.

    The results also suggest that the physical

    attractiveness variable may have a number of

    implications for a variety of aspects of social

    interaction and influence. For example, it is

    clear that physically attractive individuals

    may have even more advantages in the dating

    market than has previously been assumed. In

    addition to an aesthetic advantage in marrying

    a beautiful spouse (cf. Josselin de Jong,

    19S2), potential marriage partners may also

    assume that the beautiful attract all of the

    world’s material benefits and happiness.

    Thus, the lure of an attractive marriage partner

    should be strong indeed.

    We do not know, of course, how well this

    stereotype stands up against contradictory information.

    Nor do we know the extent to

    which it determines the pattern of social

    interaction that develops with a person of a

    particular attractiveness level. Nevertheless,

    it would be odd if people did not behave

    toward others in accordance with this stereotype.

    Such behavior has been previously

    noted anecdotally. Monahan (1941) has observed

    that

    Even social workers accustomed to dealing with all

    types often find it difficult to think of a normal,

    pretty girl as being guilty of a crime. Most people,

    for some inexplicable reason, think of crime in terms

    of abnormality in appearance, and I must say that

    beautiful women are not often convicted [p. 103].

    A host of other familiar social psychological

    dependent variables also should be affected

    in predictable ways.

    In the above connection, it might be noted

    that if standards of physical attractiveness

    vary widely, knowledge of the content of the

    physical attractiveness stereotype would be of

    limited usefulness in predicting its effect on

    social interaction and the development of the

    self-concept. The present study was not designed

    to investigate the degree of variance

    in perceived beauty. (The physical attractiveness

    ratings of the stimulus materials were

    made by college students of a similar background

    to those who participated in this

    study.) Preliminary evidence (Cross & Cross,

    1971) suggests that such differences in perceived

    beauty may not be as severe as some

    observers have suggested.

    REFERENCES

    ARONSON, E. Some antecedents of interpersonal

    attraction. In W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.),

    Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1969, 17,

    143-177.

    290 K. DION, E. BERSCHEID, AND E. WALSTER

    CROSS, J. F., & CROSS, J. Age, sex, race, and the per- MONAHAN, F. Women in crime. New York: Ives

    ception of facial beauty. Developmental Psychol- Washburn, 1941.

    ogy, 1971, 5, 433-439. SCHILLER, J. C. F. Essays, esthetical and philo-

    HAYS, W. L. Statistics for psychologists. New York: sophical, including the dissertation on the “Con-

    Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1963. nexions between the animal and the spiritual in

    JOSSELIN DE JONG, J. P. B. Levi-Strauss’ theory on man.” London: Bell, 1882.

    kinship and marriage. Leiden, Holland: Brill, 19S2. WRIGHT, B. A. Physical disability—A psychological

    KIRKPATRICK, C., & COTTON, J. Physical attractive- approach. New York: Harper & Row, 1960.

    ness, age, and marital adjustment. American Sociological

    Review, 1951, 16, 81-86. (Received July 14, 1971)

    Manuscripts Accepted for Publication in the

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    Social Distance as Categorization of Intergroup Interaction. Carolyn W. Sherif (Department of Psychology,

    Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802).

    Film-Induced Arousal, Information Search, and the Attribution Process. Michel Girodo (Royal Ottawa Hospital,

    1145 Carling Avenue, Ottawa, Ontario KIZ 7K4).

    Tngroup Norms and Self-Identity as Determinants of Discriminatory Behavior. Khor 0. Boyanowsky (Department

    of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia) and Vernon L. Allen.

    Experimentally Induced Changes in Moral Opinions and Reasoning (Charles Blake Keasey (Department of

    Psychology, Douglass College, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey 08903).

    Personality Variables Associated with Cigarette Smoking. Richard W. Coan (Department of Psychology, University

    of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721).

    Personality and Social Systems and Attitude-Reinforcer-Discriminative Theory: Interest (Attitude) Formation,

    Function, and Measurement. Arthur W. Staats (Department of Psychology, 2430 Campus Road, University

    of Hawaii, Honolulu, Hawaii 96822), Michael C. Cross, Peter F. Quay, and Carl C. Carlson.

    Rules, Models, and Self-Reinforcement in Children. David E. Hildebrant (Department of Psychology, Northern

    Illinois University, Dekalb, Illinois 60115), Solomon F. Feldman, and Raymond A. Ditrichs.

    Attitude Change and Attitude Attribution: Effects of Incentive, Choice, and Consequences in the Festinger and

    Carlsmith Paradigm. Bobby J. Calder (Department of Business Administration, University of Illinois,

    Urbana, Illinois 61801), Michael Ross, and Chester A. Insko.

    The Defendant’s Dilemma: Effects of Jurors’ Attitudes and Authoritarianism on Judicial Decisions. Herman E.

    Mitchell and Donn Byrne (Department of Psychology, Purdue University, Lafayette, Indiana 47907).

    Internal versus External Control of Reinforcement and Reaction to Frustration. Michael Brissett and Stephen

    Nowicki, Jr. (Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322).

    Attitudinal Politics: The Strategy of Moderation. Robert B. Cialdini (Department of Psychology, Arizona State

    University, Temne, Arizona 85281), Alan Levy, C. Peter Herman, and Scott Evenbeck.

    Smoking, Physiological Arousal, and Emotional Response. Paul David Nesbitt (Department of Psychology,

    University of California, Santa Barbara, California 93106).

    Investigation into Deindividuation Using a Cross-Cultural Survey Technique. Robert I. Watson, Jr. (Department

    of Social Relations, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138).

    When Do Opposites Attract? When They Are Opposite in Sex and Sex-Role Attitudes. B. A. Seyfried (Department

    of Psychology, Kent State University, Kent, Ohio 44240) and Clyde Hendrick.

    Stimulus Inconsistency and Response Dispositions in Forming Judgments of Other Persons. Martin F. Kaplan

    (Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University, Dekalb, Illinois 60115).

    Context Effects in Observed Violence. Russell G. Geen (Department of Psychology, University of Missouri,

    Columbia, Missouri 65201) and David Stonner.

    Pygmalion Black and White. Pamela C. Rubovits and Martin L. Maehr (Department of Education Psychology,

    University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois 61801).

    Exposure, Context, and Interpersonal Attraction. Susan Saegert (Environmental Psychology Program, The City

    University of New York, 33 West 42nd Street, New York, New York 10036), Walter Swap, and R. B. Zajonc.

    Power, Opportunity Costs, and Sex in a Mixed-Motive Game. Jeffrey Bedell and Frank Sistrunk (Director of

    Social Sciences, Stale University System of Florida, 107 West Gaincs Street, Tallahassee, Florida 32304).

    (Continued on fiage 305)

 

Subject Article Analysis Pages 5 Style APA

Answer

        1. Article Review: What is Beautiful is Good

          Dion, Berscheid, and Walster (1972), in the article “What is Beautiful is Good,” investigate the influence of people’s physical appearance on their social and personality psychology. Primarily, Dion, Berscheid, and Walster (1972) assume that attractive people are likely to be poorer parents due to the absence of any study which establishes a correlation between physical appearance and personality. For instance, they foster the perspective that “beauty is only a peripheral characteristic.” Moreover, the assumption is enhanced by the statement that beauty only serves as an antecedent for liking hence the folk psychology view that “what is beautiful is good.”

          Beautiful people, especially women, are considered less homely than unattractive or average attractive persons. Whereas this is only a theoretical perspective, social psychologists have not engaged in any study to substantiate these claims. As such, it remains a general belief that beautiful women are not homely hence poor parents.

          The authors’ experimental study confirmed the existence of appearance-based stereotypes when evaluating individuals of different attractiveness. Notably, there is increased likeness for attractive people as compared to those that are average or unattractive. Beautiful persons were rated as more social, while unattractive ones were least social. Basing on the significance of stereotyping, social psychologists hold the stereotyped perception that “physically attractive people are likely to poorer parents than unattractive or average attractive people” as being right despite the lack of evidence supporting its correctness.

          Critical Thinking Question

          Could the fear of disturbing the established psychological balance between being attractive and being a good parent, where individuals who are not attractive are liked for being good parents, be why social psychologists have shelved their interests in examining if beauty is not just a peripheral characteristic?

         

References

Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of personality and social psychology24(3), 285.

 

Related Samples

WeCreativez WhatsApp Support
Our customer support team is here to answer your questions. Ask us anything!
👋 Hi, how can I help?